Can the Catholic Church Help Explain Western Psychology?

A social-science analysis of how Catholicism transformed Western culture

The inside of a catholic church
     

During the past 10 years, social scientists have wrestled with a powerful criticism of their research: their favorite subjects, American college students, are often outliers compared to the global population. Economists and psychologists run studies on their students, and then sometimes use those results to make broad claims about human tolerance for risk, moral reasoning, or even the way people perceive lines on a page. But they often are just describing a W.E.I.R.D. subgroup of humanity—Western, Educated, Industrialized, Rich, and Democratic—as an influential study calls them. That subgroup represents only 12 percent of the world’s population, but a whopping 96 percent of subjects in psychological studies. 

Joseph Henrich, professor of human evolutionary biology (HEB), was lead author on a 2010 paper in Behavioral and Brain Sciences that proposed the WEIRD problem in social science. It has since received more than 5,000 citations, rocking the academic world. Henrich and his colleagues, all then at the University of British Columbia, describe in painstaking detail how WEIRD subjects differ from the global population, arguing for example that risk-averse behavior concerning money might be a recent development local to industrialized areas, rather than intrinsic to human nature. But he avoided any speculation as to why the West had such an unusual psychological makeup.

A paper published today in Science, coauthored by Henrich with Jonathan Schulz and Jonathan Beauchamp, both economists at George Mason University, and Duman Bahrami-Rad, a postdoctoral fellow in HEB’s culture, cognition, and coevolution lab, attempts to explain how Europeans came to be so atypical. In an argument fusing methods from anthropology, psychology, and history, the authors claim that the unusual levels of individualism seen in the West come in part from the emergence of the nuclear family—which is vanishingly rare outside of Europe.

Henrich had long been thinking about how families change the ways that people think. “I was influenced by my ongoing anthropological fieldwork at one of the outer islands of Fiji, where social life is still governed by strong norms of kinship, which endow everyone with a set of responsibilities, obligations, and privileges,” said Henrich in a news conference. “It seemed plausible that kinship systems, or what we call kin-based institutions, might have a big effect on people’s psychology.”

The authors’ argument goes like this. The emergence of agriculture 12,000 years ago favored societies that could work together on big projects, like growing crops. This kind of collaboration required people to be members of tightly bound social networks, strengthened by individuals who showed solidarity with one another. Families in farming societies fostered intense connection among people, because their survival depended on it: extended relatives lived under one roof, polygamy was often allowed, and people married within their own communities and families. Practices like ancestor worship and shared ownership further strengthened these bonds, both in Europe and in many farming societies around the agricultural world.

When the Catholic Church emerged, everything changed. The medieval church in western Europe promulgated unusually strict rules about families: newlyweds were often required to move to a new house, polygamy was weeded out, arranged marriages were discouraged, remarriage was banned, and legal adoption was stopped. Above all, the church harshly condemned incest. People were forbidden to marry their sixth cousins or anyone closer, as well as in-laws and “spiritual relatives” like godparents. Priests and elders would do background checks before the ceremony to uncover any hidden overlap in family trees. The motivations for these new rules are somewhat unclear, but church fathers like Ambrose and Augustine condemned incest in their writings, and various early councils saw it as an affront to God.

“I was surprised just how preoccupied medieval Europe was with the fear of incest,” said Schulz. “Natural disasters such as the plague were attributed to God’s punishment for incest.” When ministers at weddings ask if anyone has an objection to a couple tying the knot, they are drawing on a tradition that was designed to catch incestuous marriages before they happened.

“Interestingly, none of these things can be found in the Bible,” said Henrich at the news conference. “The Western Church bumbled on to a particular combination of taboos and prescriptions and prohibitions.”

The church’s marriage rules resulted in nuclear households, weak family ties, and late marriage, which the authors believe have had deep and lasting psychological impacts on Europeans and their descendants. Unlike the solidarity and conformity that might be favored in earlier agricultural society, Christianized Western Europe favored more individualism. Even though the strict marriage taboos began to loosen in the thirteenth century and the continent has since secularized, the authors thought this lasting change to family structures might explain the peculiar psychology of the WEIRD. 

To test this hypothesis, Schulz and colleagues did a massive data analysis. They used medieval documents recording where bishops moved to track the expansion of Catholicism, mapping the length of time that each population in the world has been exposed to the Western Church. They then designed a metric to measure how tightly knit family structures were, including information about cousin marriage rates and whether people lived with extended family. Finally, they made a metric of WEIRDness using current psychological studies and surveys. They found that the more times a population was exposed to the Western Church, the less strong their extended family networks were and the more independence they showed. (The Eastern church had similar but smaller impacts on people.) To make sure this pattern resulted from exposure to the church, the researchers controlled for a large number of variables, including environmental conditions, the presence of medieval universities, and the number of Roman roads through the region.

Even before this new study, some anthropologists have been wary of the possibility of assigning a sense of superiority to the WEIRD. “I worry that W.E.I.R.D. classification flatters the WEIRD, focusing on traits that Westerners typically highlight to describe themselves in ways that are, however inadvertently, pretty self-congratulatory,” wrote anthropologist Greg Downey when the original WEIRD article came out. “If we were to call the same group, Materialist, Young, self-Obsessed, Pleasure-seeking, Isolated, Consumerist, and Sedentary (MYOPICS)… you get the idea.”

This study will surely raise similar objections. In the paper, the WEIRD psychology is associated with “individualism,” “creativity,” “independence,” “analytical thinking,” and trust toward strangers, while the groups with more intense family structures are associated with “conformity,” “obedience,” and “nepotism.” One of the metrics the authors use to measure the amount of socialness a population shows to those outside the immediate community is the number of parking tickets a given country’s United Nations diplomats get in New York City. Because diplomats have immunity from local prosecution, they do not have to pay traffic tickets and can theoretically park wherever they want without punishment. The idea is that the more often a nation’s diplomats take advantage of this privilege—by parking in no-parking zones or fire lanes and not paying the fines, the less “impersonally prosocial” that nation is. All this might feed into a sense of European superiority, based not in biology but in Christian roots. 

“We’re not claiming that there’s a better configuration in one kin institution or the other,” said Beauchamp, adding that strong extended families provide a kind of safety net. “We’re just establishing a statistical link here.” Henrich also pointed out that their model explains variation in psychology within Europe.

Anthropologist Jared Diamond ’58, JF ’65, author of Guns, Germs, and Steel, has been criticized by some anthropologists for his “environmental determinism”: the idea that climate and geography determine the success of civilizations. This paper has a much more robust analysis than Diamond’s, but still rests on a single factor: the church. There is a deterministic feel to the work—for example, the authors show that second-generation immigrants to Europe perform differently on psychological tests than the native-born population does, predicted in part by the family structures of their ancestral country.

Attempts to explain the WEIRD won’t stop anytime soon. As one reviewer described the work: “This is the paper that launches a thousand ships.”

Read more articles by Drew Pendergrass

You might also like

Harvard Alumnus Wins Chemistry Nobel

David Baker ’84 invents new proteins not found in nature.

American Jewish Life After October 7

Professors Derek Penslar and Noah Feldman reflect on a difficult year

Gary Ruvkun Shares Nobel Prize in Medicine

Harvard Medical School genetics professor honored  

Most popular

Why Do We Still Have the Electoral College?

Historian Alexander Keyssar on why the unpopular institution has prevailed 

Harvard Alumnus Wins Chemistry Nobel

David Baker ’84 invents new proteins not found in nature.

The Prison Problem

Sociologist Bruce Western rethinks incarceration in America

More to explore

Learning the Trees of North America

A monumental new guide to North American species

An Underknown Twentieth Century Realist Artist

Brief life of an American realist artist and critic: 1907-1975

Susan Farbstein on Human Rights Law

Human rights lawyer on law’s ability to promote justice—and shape public understanding